This Is Your Brain on Drug Education

fried eggs

By Carl Hart @drcarlhart

A man holds up an egg and says, “This is your brain.”  Then, he picks up a frying pan and says, “This is drugs.” Then, he cracks open the egg, fries the contents, and says, “This is your brain on drugs.” Finally, he asks, “Any questions?” This memorable anti-drug use advertisement served as “drug education” in the 1980s.

Unfortunately, today’s drug education isn’t much better. One of the most popular anti-drug use advertisements is sponsored by the Montana Meth Project. In general, these

advertisements show, in horrifying detail, a young person who uses methamphetamine for the first time and then ends up engaging in some unthinkable act such as prostitution or assaulting strangers for money to buy the drug. At the end of advertisement, printed on screen is “Meth, not even once.”

While the goal of decreasing drug abuse is a commendable one, these types of media campaigns have been shown to have no effect on drug use or abuse (Anderson, 2010). Furthermore, they do not disseminate any real facts about drug effects, but they go a long way to perpetuate false assumptions about illegal drug use and their effects. Persistent misconceptions about crack cocaine and methamphetamine are that the drugs have a unique propensity to produce dangerous effects. In the 1980s (and even now), crack cocaine, for example, was portrayed as producing unpredictable and deadly effects. And today, methamphetamine is said to cause neurotoxicity and widespread cognitive impairments.

Over the past 15 years, I have given thousands of doses of drugs such as crack cocaine and methamphetamine to people. I do this, with the informed consent of the research participant, as part of my research to understand how drugs affect the brain, behavior and physiology. Based on my studies and data from other researchers, the effects produced by crack and powder cocaine are identical; they are also predictable. As the dose is increased, so are the effects. Methamphetamine, too, produces predictable effects. Indeed, the effects produced by methamphetamine are identical to those produced by d-amphetamine – the main compound in the popular Attention-Deficit Hyperactive Disorder (ADHD) treatment medication, Adderall (Kirkpatrick et al., 2012). The notion that methamphetamine users have brain deficits that lead to cognitive impairments is not supported by evidence from research (Hart et al., 2012).

The bottom line is that many of the immediate and long-term harmful effects caused by methamphetamine use have been greatly exaggerated, just as the dangers of crack cocaine were overstated nearly three decades earlier(Hart, 2013).

As a result, I contend that the exaggeration of harmful effects produced by crack cocaine and methamphetamine has helped shape a political environment in which there is an unwarranted and unrealistic goal of eliminating the use of certain drugs at any cost to specific users, i.e., the poor and minorities. For example, despite the fact that there was virtually no evidence supporting claims that crack produces unique destructive effects, in the late 1980s, the U.S. Congress passed the now infamous Anti-Drug Abuse Act, setting 100 times harsher penalties for crack than for powder cocaine convictions.

A whopping 85 percent of those sentenced for crack offenses are black, even though the majority of users of the drug are white (USSC, 2007).

Similar racial disparities have been reported for marijuana arrests (ACLU, 2013). And now, largely because of the selective targeting of black males by drug enforcement agencies, one in three black boys born today will spend time in prison if we don’t alter our current approach (Bonczar, 2003).

So what can be done? 

    • Funding agencies could help remedy this situation by requesting research applications that focus explicitly on race—for example, trying to understand the long-term consequences of drug arrests on black people, especially as they relate to disrupting one’s life trajectory.
    • Scientists who study drugs of abuse can contribute by widening their scope such that they examine and report a broader range of drug effects and not focus exclusively on drug-related pathology.
    • Policy makers should consider drug decriminalization, in which drug sales remain illegal, but drug possessions do not lead to criminal convictions—the one thing that has prevented so many from obtaining employment, housing, governmental benefits, treatment, etc. This is crucial because each year, more than 80 percent of drug arrests involve only simple possession (FBI, 2010).

Not only am I a scientist, but I am also the father of three black sons. I recognize that there is a high probability that they, like their white counterparts and our three most recent Presidents, may one day experiment with drugs. Knowing the potential consequences if they are arrested, I cannot afford to remain silent.

Dr. Hart recently gave a presentation at APA as part of our Health Disparities seminar series entitled: Challenging the Assumptions about Drug Abuse.  You can watch it below:

 

Biography

Dr. Carl Hart is an Associate Professor in the Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry at Columbia University. He is also a Research Scientist in the Division of Substance Abuse at the New York State Psychiatric Institute. A major focus of Dr. Hart’s research is to understand complex interactions between drugs of abuse and the neurobiology and environmental factors that mediate human behavior and physiology. Hart is a member of the National Advisory Council on Drug Abuse and on the board of directors of the College on Problems of Drug Dependence and the Drug Policy Alliance. His most recent book, High Price is a complex story of scientific achievement in the face of overwhelming odds; it also highlights that U.S. drug policy is based on many false assumptions and the enforcement of such policies is racially biased.

References

American Civil Liberties Union (ACLU) (2013). The war on marijuana in black and white. New York, NY: Author. Retrieved from: https://www.aclu.org/criminal-law-reform/war-marijuana-black-and-white-report

Anderson, D. M. (2010). Does information matter? The effect of the Meth Project on meth use among youths. Journal of Health Economics, 29, 732-42.

Bonczar, T.P., (2003). Prevalence of imprisonment in the U.S. population, 1974-2001. U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics Special Report, NCJ 197976. Retrieved from: www.policyalmanac.org/crime/archive/prisoners_in_US_pop.pdf.

Federal Bureau of Investigation (2010). Crime in the United States, 2010. Washington, DC: Author. Retrieved from http://www.fbi.gov/about-us/cjis/ucr/crime-in-the-u.s/2010/crime-in-the-u.s.-2010/persons-arrested

Hart,C. L., Marvin,C. B., Silver,R., Smith, E. E. (2012). Is cognitive functioning impaired in methamphetamine users? A critical review. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37, 586-608.

Hart, C. L. (2013) High Price: A neuroscientist’s journey of self-discovery that challenges everything you know about drugs and society. Harper-Collins: New York.

Kirkpatrick, M. G., Gunderson, E. W., Johanson, C. E., Levin, F. R., Foltin, R. W., Hart, C. L. (2012) Comparison of intranasal methamphetamine and d-amphetamine self-administration by humans. Addiction, 107, 783-91.

U.S. Sentencing Commission (2007). Report to the Congress: Cocaine and federal sentencing policy. Washington, DC: Author. Retrieved from: http://www.ussc.gov/Legislative_and_Public_Affairs/Congressional_Testimony_and_Reports/Drug_Topics/200705_RtC_Cocaine_Sentencing_Policy.pdf

Reposted From: psychologybenefits.org